-
Notifications
You must be signed in to change notification settings - Fork 2
/
plc38-presentation.tex
379 lines (318 loc) · 13.5 KB
/
plc38-presentation.tex
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
61
62
63
64
65
66
67
68
69
70
71
72
73
74
75
76
77
78
79
80
81
82
83
84
85
86
87
88
89
90
91
92
93
94
95
96
97
98
99
100
101
102
103
104
105
106
107
108
109
110
111
112
113
114
115
116
117
118
119
120
121
122
123
124
125
126
127
128
129
130
131
132
133
134
135
136
137
138
139
140
141
142
143
144
145
146
147
148
149
150
151
152
153
154
155
156
157
158
159
160
161
162
163
164
165
166
167
168
169
170
171
172
173
174
175
176
177
178
179
180
181
182
183
184
185
186
187
188
189
190
191
192
193
194
195
196
197
198
199
200
201
202
203
204
205
206
207
208
209
210
211
212
213
214
215
216
217
218
219
220
221
222
223
224
225
226
227
228
229
230
231
232
233
234
235
236
237
238
239
240
241
242
243
244
245
246
247
248
249
250
251
252
253
254
255
256
257
258
259
260
261
262
263
264
265
266
267
268
269
270
271
272
273
274
275
276
277
278
279
280
281
282
283
284
285
286
287
288
289
290
291
292
293
294
295
296
297
298
299
300
301
302
303
304
305
306
307
308
309
310
311
312
313
314
315
316
317
318
319
320
321
322
323
324
325
326
327
328
329
330
331
332
333
334
335
336
337
338
339
340
341
342
343
344
345
346
347
348
349
350
351
352
353
354
355
356
357
358
359
360
361
362
363
364
365
366
367
368
369
370
371
372
373
374
375
376
377
378
379
% Copyright 2013 Aaron Ecay and Meredith Tamminga
% Available under the Creative Commons BY-SA or GPL v2+ licenses: see
% the file LICENSE for more information
\documentclass{digs-slides}
\usepackage{expex}
\usepackage{multicol}
\usepackage{amssymb}
% \bibliographystyle{linquiry2}
\usetikzlibrary{calc}
\newcommand{\includegraph}[2][]{\mode<beamer>{\includegraphics[#1]{#2}}
\mode<handout>{\includegraphics[#1]{#2-handout}}}
\addbibresource{biblio.bib}
\title{Priming as a diagnostic of grammatical status: Two case studies}
\author{Meredith Tamminga and Aaron Ecay}
\institute{University of Pennsylvania}
\date{Mar.\ 29, 2014 \\\vspace{0.5em} Penn Linguistics Conference 38}
\begin{document}
\begin{frame}
\titlepage
\end{frame}
\section*{Introduction}
\subsection*{foo}
\begin{frame}{Introduction}
\begin{itemize}
\item Diachronic generative syntax encompasses the analysis both
of historical grammatical structures and of the processes by
which they change
\item Analysis of underlying structures is particularly challenging
without access to native speakers
\end{itemize}
\end{frame}
\begin{frame}<beamer>
\frametitle{Introduction}
\begin{itemize}
\item Researchers have made headway by using the Constant Rate
Effect \parencite{Kroch1989} to infer grammatical
analyses through quantitative data on historical change
\item We will propose an independent source of quantitative
evidence about historical grammatical analyses: clustering
tendencies across tokens
\end{itemize}
\end{frame}
\begin{frame}
\frametitle{Table of contents}
\tableofcontents{}
\end{frame}
\section{Background on priming}
\subsection*{foo}
\begin{frame}{Priming}
\begin{itemize}
\item \textbf{Priming:} the tendency to reuse a recently
encountered linguistic option
\item Extensive structural priming
literature \parencite[beginning with][]{Bock:1986}
demonstrates that syntactic structures can be primed
\item Example:
\begin{itemize}
\item \textbf{Prime:} I gave the children candy
\item[$\Uparrow$] I gave the dog treats
\item[$\Downarrow$] I gave treats to the dog
\end{itemize}
\end{itemize}
\end{frame}
\begin{frame}{Priming reflects structural identity}
\begin{itemize}
\item \textcite{Estival:1985}: different types of passives (lexical vs.\ transformational) each prime themselves but not each other
\item The structural distinction this reflects is maintained in modern syntactic accounts \parencite[e.g.][]{Embick:2004}
\end{itemize}
% Code for this graph:
% > estival.graph2(write = TRUE)
\begin{center}
\includegraph{figures/estival}
\end{center}
\end{frame}
\begin{frame}{Priming reflects structural identity}
\begin{itemize}
\item \textcite{Bock:1990}: Infinitival purpose clauses with
“to” do not prime prepositional datives with “to”
\begin{itemize}
\item I brought a book to study
\item I brought a book to Stella
\end{itemize}
\item \textcite{Ferreira:2003}: complementizer \textit{that}
presence is not increased by previous use of demonstrative \textit{that}
\end{itemize}
\end{frame}
\begin{frame}{Priming reflects structural identity}
\begin{itemize}
\item Priming can be a useful dependent variable for its reflection of underlying structures: repetition reveals sameness
\item “If the processing of a stimulus affects the processing of another stimulus, then the two stimuli must be related [...] if the relationship between the two stimuli is syntactic, then we can use this relationship as a way of understanding what syntactic information is represented” \parencite[490]{Branigan:1995}
\item Linking hypothesis: priming effects in written
historical data reflect structural identity in
language production at the time
\end{itemize}
\end{frame}
\section{Case one: Negation}
\subsection{Empirical description}
\label{sec:empirical-aspects}
\begin{frame}{The change in negation}
\begin{itemize}
\item In Middle English, there is a change in the exponence of Neg
\item The negator \emph{ne}, inherited from OE, is lost
\item \emph{not}, formerly a negative adverb, becomes the new negator
\end{itemize}
\end{frame}
\begin{frame}{Details of the change}
\begin{itemize}
\item During the period of the change, a large number of negative
sentences have both \emph{ne} and \emph{not}:
\end{itemize}
\ex~
he ne shal nouʒt decieue him \trailingcitation{Early Prose Psalter,
161:131:11, from \textcite{Frisch1997}}
\xe
% To generate this plot:
% > neg <- cleanNegData("queries/coding.cod.ooo")
% > three.lines.graph2(neg, write = TRUE)
\begin{center}
\includegraph{figures/three-lines2}
\end{center}
\end{frame}
\subsection{Two analyses}
\label{sec:analyses}
\begin{frame}{Two analyses}
\begin{multicols}{2}
\begin{center}
\textcite{Frisch1997}
\begin{tikzpicture}[scale=3]
\node[circle, draw, minimum size=0.6in](ne-atom) at (1,1) {\textsc{ne}};
\node[circle, draw, minimum size=0.6in](not-atom) at (1,0) {\textsc{not}};
\node[align=left](ne-surface) at (0,1) {\emph{ne}};
\node[align=left](ne-not-surface) at (0,0.5) {\emph{ne ... not}};
\node[align=left](not-surface) at (0,0) {\emph{not}};
\draw[<-,>=triangle 45] (ne-atom) -- (ne-surface);
\draw[<-,>=triangle 45] (ne-atom) -- (ne-not-surface);
\draw[<-,>=triangle 45] (not-atom) -- (not-surface);
\draw[<-,>=triangle 45] (not-atom) -- (ne-not-surface);
\end{tikzpicture}
\end{center}
\begin{center}
\textcite{wallage08}
\begin{tikzpicture}[scale=3]
\node[circle, draw, minimum size=0.55in](ne-atom) at (1,1) {\textsc{ne}};
\node[circle, draw, minimum size=0.55in](ne-not-atom) at (1,0.5) {\textsc{ne-not}};
\node[circle, draw, minimum size=0.55in](not-atom) at (1,0) {\textsc{not}};
\node[align=left](ne-surface) at (0,1) {\emph{ne}};
\node[align=left](ne-not-surface) at (0,0.5) {\emph{ne ... not}};
\node[align=left](not-surface) at (0,0) {\emph{not}};
\draw[<-,>=triangle 45] (ne-atom) -- (ne-surface);
\draw[<-,>=triangle 45] (ne-not-atom) -- (ne-not-surface);
\draw[<-,>=triangle 45] (not-atom) -- (not-surface);
\end{tikzpicture}
\end{center}
\end{multicols}
\end{frame}
\begin{frame}
\frametitle{Results (PPCME2)}
\begin{multicols}{2}
% > nnb.fac.graph2(neg, write = TRUE)
\includegraph[width=\linewidth]{figures/ne-not2}
% > patch.graph2(neg, write = TRUE)
\includegraph[width=\linewidth]{figures/patch2}
\end{multicols}
\end{frame}
\section{Case two: \emph{do}-support}
\label{sec:case-two:-emphdo}
\subsection{Empirical description}
\label{sec:empir-descr}
\begin{frame}
\frametitle{\emph{Do}-support}
\begin{itemize}
\item In the Early Modern English (EME) period (1500–1700),
the use of auxiliary \emph{do} became obligatory in English
negative declaratives and questions (inter alia)
\item During the course of this change there was a period when
\emph{do} was used in (non-emphatic) affirmative declaratives,
which is banned in present-day English
\end{itemize}
\includegraphics[width=\textwidth]{static-figures/do-all}
\end{frame}
\begin{frame}
\frametitle{Two kinds of \emph{do}}
\begin{itemize}
\item \textcite{Ecay2012a} proposed that the \emph{do} found in
affirmative declaratives is distinct from the modern type of
\emph{do}, is merged lower than T, and functions in early EME as
a marker of argument structure
\end{itemize}
\includegraphics[width=\textwidth]{static-figures/do-aff}
\begin{itemize}
\item If this is so, it should be visible in priming data
\end{itemize}
\end{frame}
\subsection{Predictions}
\label{sec:predictions}
\begin{frame}
\frametitle{Priming predictions for \emph{do}}
\begin{itemize}
\item The intermediate (low-\emph{do}) grammar can produce surface
\emph{do} in affirmative declarative contexts
\item It can \textbf{also} do so in modern \emph{do}-support
contexts (with external argument)
\item The modern (high-\emph{do}) grammar can produce surface
\emph{do} only in the modern \emph{do}-support contexts
% \item Thus, three types of grammatical objects: affirmative
% declarative produced by intermediate grammar, modern-type do
% produced by intermediate grammar, and modern-type do produced by
% modern grammar
\end{itemize}
\end{frame}
\begin{frame}
\begin{center}
\begin{tikzpicture}[xscale=1.55]
\node[circle,draw,minimum size=0.75in] (v1) at (-3.5,4.5) {Intermed.};
\node[circle,draw,minimum size=0.75in] (v2) at (0.5,4.5) {Intermed.};
\node[circle,draw,minimum size=0.75in,visible on=<2>] (v4) at (-3.5,2) {Intermed.};
\node[circle,draw,minimum size=0.75in,visible on=<2>] (v3) at (0.5,2) {Intermed.};
\node[circle,draw,minimum size=0.75in] (v5) at (-3.5,-0.5) {Modern};
\node[circle,draw,minimum size=0.75in] (v6) at (0.5,-0.5) {Modern};
\draw ($(v4.north west)+(-0.1in,0.15in)$) rectangle ($(v5.south east)+(0.1in,-0.15in)$);
\draw ($(v3.north west)+(-0.1in,0.15in)$) rectangle ($(v6.south east)+(0.1in,-0.15in)$);
\draw ($(v2.north west)+(-0.1in,0.15in)$) rectangle ($(v2.south east)+(0.1in,-0.15in)$);
\draw ($(v1.north west)+(-0.1in,0.15in)$) rectangle ($(v1.south east)+(0.1in,-0.15in)$);
\node[draw] at (-1.5,5) {Affirmative declarative};
\node[draw] at (-1.5,0.5) {Modern contexts};
\draw [->,>=triangle 45] (v1) edge (v2);
\draw [->,>=triangle 45,visible on=<2>] (v1) edge (v3);
\draw [->,>=triangle 45,visible on=<2>] (v4) edge (v2);
\draw [->,>=triangle 45,visible on=<2>] (v4) edge (v3);
\draw [->,>=triangle 45] (v5) edge (v6);
\node at (-3.5,6) {\textbf{Prime}};
\node at (0.5,6) {\textbf{Target}};
\end{tikzpicture}
\end{center}
\end{frame}
\subsection{Results}
\label{sec:results}
\begin{frame}
\frametitle{Results}
% > source("scripts/dosupp.R")
\includegraph[width=\textwidth]{figures/do-results}
\end{frame}
\begin{frame}<handout>
\frametitle{Results (PPCEME + PCEEC)}
\begin{itemize}
\item Affirmative declaratives always prime each other, as expected
\item Modern environments prime each other more strongly over
time, as disguised affirmative declaratives disappear
\item Cross-priming lessens over time, also related to the
disappearance of disguised affirmative declaratives
\end{itemize}
\end{frame}
\section{Conclusions}
\label{sec:conclusion}
\subsection*{foo}
\begin{frame}{Conclusions}
\begin{itemize}
\item The Constant Rate Effect is important because it
provides a link between frequency data attested in historical
corpora and the mental representations that underlie language
and language change
\item We would like to suggest that priming data constitute
another, independent source of linkage between these two domains
\begin{itemize}
\item As has been shown in our two case studies
\end{itemize}
\item The investigation of priming evidence can support and refine
the conclusions of quantitative studies of syntactic change
\end{itemize}
\end{frame}
\begin{frame}
\frametitle{Conclusions}
\begin{itemize}
\item Priming research has been deployed in studies of spoken
corpora to answer questions about grammatical variation in
contemporary American English and Romance languages
\item There is also a large and growing experimental literature on
structural priming, to which this work creates obvious bridges
\item We would love to see more linguists using priming methods as
a tool to understand their favorite variables!
\end{itemize}
\end{frame}
\appendix{}
\section{Acknowledgments}
\subsection{foo}
\begin{frame}
\frametitle{Acknowledgments}
We would like to thank the following:
\begin{itemize}
\item The compilers of the corpora used (PPCME2, PPCEME, PCEEC)
\item Beatrice Santorini
\item Tony Kroch
\item Our fellow graduate students at Penn
\item The audience of DiGS15 for comments on an earlier version
(containing the negation results)
\end{itemize}
\end{frame}
\begin{frame}
\frametitle{High technology}
All the data and code used in this analysis is available on GitHub:
\url{https://github.com/aecay/digs15-negative-priming}
\mode<beamer>{\includegraphics[height=3in]{figures/github}}
\end{frame}
\begin{frame}<beamer>
\frametitle{Questions}
\begin{center}
\Huge
Questions?
\end{center}
\end{frame}
\section{Bibliography}
\label{sec:bibliography}
\subsection*{foo}
\begin{frame}[allowframebreaks=0.9]{Bibliography}
\printbibliography[heading=none]
\end{frame}
% TODO: put details about the negation patch in bonus slides section
\end{document}